Affect Regulation Schore Free Pdf Download
Different theoretical models have been used to understand and treat psychopathology. This article organizes these different models into two categories: affect regulation and affect reconsolidation. Affect regulation focuses on the management of emotion and related symptoms, whereas affect reconsolidation attempts to change underlying emotions. Historically, and often still today a classification can be made between theoretical models of psychotherapy as having either affect regulation or an affect reconsolidation approach in response to emotion in psychotherapy. Distinctions between affect regulation and affect reconsolidation therapeutic interventions are explored, with an emphasis on grounding these two approaches in affective neuroscience. The article demonstrates how affect regulation and affect reconsolidation are most effective when used together in the comprehensive treatment of many psychological disorders. Research indicates that emotion should be activated and regulated to reach a moderate level of arousal in creating the optimal conditions for affect reconsolidation to occur. Additionally, understanding how the principles of affect regulation and affect reconsolidation complement each other is essential in integrating psychotherapy and advancing our understanding of what a transtheoretical model of treatment might look like.
Discover the world's research
- 20+ million members
- 135+ million publications
- 700k+ research projects
Join for free
Journal of Psychotherapy Integration
Affect Regulation and Affect Reconsolidation as
Organizing Principles in Psychotherapy
Francis L. Stevens
Online First Publication, October 4, 2018. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/int0000130
CITATION
Stevens, F. L. (2018, October 4). Affect Regulation and Affect Reconsolidation as Organizing
Principles in Psychotherapy. Journal of Psychotherapy Integration. Advance online publication.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/int0000130
Affect Regulation and Affect Reconsolidation as Organizing
Principles in Psychotherapy
Francis L. Stevens
Reliant Medical Group, Worcester, Massachusetts
Different theoretical models have been used to understand and treat psychopathology.
This article organizes these different models into two categories: affect regulation and
affect reconsolidation. Affect regulation focuses on the management of emotion and
related symptoms, whereas affect reconsolidation attempts to change underlying emo-
tions. Historically, and often still today a classification can be made between theoretical
models of psychotherapy as having either affect regulation or an affect reconsolidation
approach in response to emotion in psychotherapy. Distinctions between affect regu-
lation and affect reconsolidation therapeutic interventions are explored, with an em-
phasis on grounding these two approaches in affective neuroscience. The article
demonstrates how affect regulation and affect reconsolidation are most effective when
used together in the comprehensive treatment of many psychological disorders. Re-
search indicates that emotion should be activated and regulated to reach a moderate
level of arousal in creating the optimal conditions for affect reconsolidation to occur.
Additionally, understanding how the principles of affect regulation and affect recon-
solidation complement each other is essential in integrating psychotherapy and advanc-
ing our understanding of what a transtheoretical model of treatment might look like.
Keywords: affect regulation, affect reconsolidation, salience network, affective neuro-
science, psychotherapy integration
Throughout the history of clinical psychol-
ogy, different theoretical models have been de-
veloped and various approaches have been
taken to understand psychopathology and guide
treatment. Early psychoanalysis centered on ac-
cessing the unconscious. This was followed by
a focus on behavior in understanding psychopa-
thology in the absence of mental processes, and
later an emphasis was placed on cognition in
determining feelings and behaviors. Histori-
cally, emotion has often been ignored in the
etiology of psychopathology because of its ab-
stract nature (Mennin & Farach, 2007). Yet
recently, emotion has become better defined and
more empirically supported (Gross & Jazaieri,
2014) as the field of affective science has
emerged. Affective science places importance
on emotion in diagnosis and treatment, and of-
fers a way to integrate the field of clinical psy-
chology (Gross, 1998; Tracy, Klonsky, &
Proudfit, 2014). Affective science has therefore
offered the field of clinical psychology a way to
assimilate and synthesize the previous frag-
mented camps of clinical psychology (Rotten-
berg & Johnson, 2007; Sloan et al., 2017). In
this article, affect regulation and affect recon-
solidation, tenets of affective science, are pro-
posed as organizing principles for psychother-
apy treatment in the field of clinical psychology.
Affect regulation, also referred to as emotion
regulation, is the ability to effectively manage
and tolerate emotions (Aldao, Nolen-Hoek-
sema, & Schweizer, 2010; Gross, 1998). Affect
regulation involves coping with negative emo-
tion without engaging in destructive or self-
defeating behaviors (Ochsner & Gross, 2005;
Schore, 2016). However, affect regulation may
also include managing positive emotions, as in
the case of a manic episode or the use of sub-
Correspondence concerning this article should be ad-
dressed to Francis L. Stevens, Department of Behavioral
Medicine, Reliant Medical Group, 425 Lake Avenue North,
Worcester, MA 01605. E-mail: fstevens377@gmail.com
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Journal of Psychotherapy Integration
© 2018 American Psychological Association 2018, Vol. 1, No. 999, 000
1053-0479/18/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/int0000130
1
stances as a way to enhance a positive state of
affect (Cooper, Frone, Russell, & Mudar, 1995).
Affect reconsolidation or emotion reconsolida-
tion, originally referred to as memory reconsoli-
dation, was a phenomenon first studied in the
rodent population, in which if a memory be-
came reactivated, it also became labile and thus
subject to change (for review, see Nader &
Hardt, 2009). Further research has indicated that
this phenomenon also occurs in humans and can
be applied to changing emotional memories
(Schiller & Phelps, 2011; Schwabe, Nader, &
Pruessner, 2014). Although accessing and re-
processing emotional memories from the past
was previously identified in clinical psychol-
ogy, and referred to as catharsis oracorrective
emotional experience (Alexander & French,
1946; Bridges, 2006), only recently has clinical
psychology been able to understand the neuro-
science behind this phenomenon. The rest of
this article will outline how affect regulation
and affect reconsolidation can be used as orga-
nizing principles to better conceptualize and
unify psychotherapy interventions based in af-
fective neuroscience.
For most individuals, emotions are typically
ephemeral states brief in duration (Verduyn &
Lavrijsen, 2015), and although they can be un-
comfortable at times, for the most part, individ-
uals are able to effectively recognize and man-
age their emotions. Yet for those struggling
with mental health concerns, emotions can often
be overly negative in nature, continuous, and
often unmanageable (Rottenberg & Johnson,
2007). Disrupted states of emotion are consis-
tently observed in psychiatric diagnoses (Berk-
ing & Wupperman, 2012; Werner & Gross,
2010). Many individuals with mental health dis-
orders show an increased sensitivity to negative
affect (Daros, Uliaszek, & Ruocco, 2014).
These individuals are hypersensitive to cues of
negative feelings and are quicker to recognize
negative emotions (Bourke, Douglas, & Porter,
2010). They become hypersensitive to the in-
duction of negative stimuli such as rejection
(Cardi, Di Matteo, Corfield, & Treasure, 2013),
sadness cues (Gotlib, Krasnoperova, Yue, &
Joormann, 2004), or fear stimuli (Bar-Haim,
Lamy, Pergamin, Bakermans-Kranenburg, &
van IJzendoorn, 2007). This article proposes
that emotional difficulties are present in, and
central to, most psychological disorders (Gross
& Jazaieri, 2014).
Unifying Therapy
Proponents of different therapeutic modali-
ties have advocated for different approaches to
handling emotion in psychotherapy. Histori-
cally, for example, cognitive approaches fo-
cused on decreasing emotion, whereas experi-
ential and psychodynamic approaches focused
on increasing emotion (Mennin & Farach, 2007;
Wiser & Goldfried, 1993). Often in research
studies, cognitive– behavioral therapy (CBT)
and psychodynamic therapy are pitted against
one another in a comparison of outcomes, pre-
sumably to help clinicians identify the best
treatment approach (Driessen et al., 2017;
Leichsenring et al., 2009; Shedler, 2010). This
dichotomy between treatments could be seen as
having either an affect regulation or affect re-
consolidation approach to psychotherapy. Un-
derstanding how these differing approaches use
affect regulation or affect reconsolidation in
treatment may help in developing more inte-
grated approaches to psychotherapy.
Affect regulation involves managing emo-
tions to prevent further upsetting states of neg-
ative affect and is especially important in pre-
venting harmful behaviors, such as substance
abuse or suicide, that result from negative states
of affect. Traditional cognitive therapy regu-
lates negative affect by changing one's
thoughts. Changing negative cognitions to more
positive thoughts helps to prevent the reinforce-
ment of negative ideas about the self, thus ter-
minating a continual cascade of negative feel-
ings (Beck, Liese, & Najavits, 2005). Other
models of affect regulation include dialectic
behavioral therapy, which emphasizes mindful-
ness and the use of coping strategies to manage
overwhelming emotions (Robins, Ivanoff, &
Linehan, 2001). Affect regulation involves
learning how to control upsetting feelings and
manage thinking to prevent escalating states of
negative affect. The focus of these affect regu-
lation techniques involves mitigating excessive
unpleasant states of affect.
Affect reconsolidation exists as a way to
change emotion to prevent future experiences of
continued negative affect. Traditional behav-
ioral therapy offers an example of affect recon-
solidation as a therapeutic approach. Behavioral
therapy exposes the individual to the feared
stimulus as part of a reconditioning process.
This process aims to eliminate the feared re-
2 STEVENS
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
sponse through reexposure to the stimulus in the
absence of the fear response, often referred to as
extinction. Some debate exists in the distinction
between extinction and memory reconsolida-
tion; however, each has different mechanistic
properties (Duvarci, Mamou, & Nader, 2006).
In the psychology literature, extinction gener-
ally refers to the elimination of a conditioned
fear, through exposure to the feared stimulus,
whereas affect (memory) reconsolidation in-
volves a single instance in which the memory is
reactivated and the emotional response to the
stimulus changes. Welling (2012) identified
three major differences between exposure
through extinction and memory reconsolida-
tion: First, memory reconsolidation is used for
all negative emotions, not solely fear; second,
the coactivated state in memory reconsolidation
could involve various positive emotions, not
just relaxation; and third, in memory reconsoli-
dation, the negative stimuli is often internal and
overly present, whereas in exposure, the fear
stimuli is external and is often avoided.
Psychodynamic models of therapy typically
tend to focus less on affect regulation strategies
and more on recollection of past events. Tradi-
tionally, psychodynamic treatment attributed in-
sight as the mechanism for change, yet more
recent dynamic models focus on the reconsoli-
dation of affect as the major mechanism of
change (Fosha, 2005). These approaches at-
tempt to activate stored emotion-laden memo-
ries for reconsolidation of that emotion. More
recent therapeutic approaches, such as emotion-
focused therapy (EFT) or accelerated experien-
tial-dynamic therapy (AEDP), also focus on ac-
tivating and reconsolidating emotion (Fosha,
2002; Greenberg, 2004), and these approaches
have been increasingly recognized in the liter-
ature (Lane, Ryan, Nadel, & Greenberg, 2015).
Similar to behavioral methods, these therapies
both involve exposure to difficult emotions. Yet
with behavioral therapy, the emotion is acti-
vated via an in vivo stimulus, whereas in EFT or
AEDP, the emotion is internal and can often
involve an emotional recollection of a previ-
ously repressed or denied emotion. Other mo-
dalities of therapy have taken different ap-
proaches to psychotherapy, such as a focus on
the therapeutic relationship in interpersonal
therapy or on the body in somatic therapy, but
these therapies, too, and most all other thera-
pies, generally involve activation and/or abate-
ment of emotion.
This article highlights one clear dilemma
among therapeutic approaches: Should psycho-
therapy focus on activation or abatement of
emotion? Focusing on past emotional events
will likely increase emotion, whereas a focus on
thoughts can refocus the client away from past
negative feelings (Wiser & Goldfried, 1993).
Therefore, the question becomes whether the
role of the therapist is one of heightening feel-
ings or mitigating feelings? Increasing feelings
is necessary for affect reconsolidation, whereas
affect regulation focuses on mitigating feelings.
This assumption—that therapy should primarily
focus treatment around either increasing or mit-
igating feelings— creates a false dichotomy and
has left clinical psychology as a fractured sci-
ence, which further contributes to skepticism
because of the lack of a consistent theoretical
basis for psychotherapy. Single-sided treatment
approaches focusing only on one side of this
dichotomy are incomplete. Solely employing
affect regulation strategies is a symptom-based
model of treatment with no acknowledgment of
the potential cause of such negative emotions.
However, solely focusing on increasing the
level of emotion, important for affect reconsoli-
dation, provides the client with no skills to
manage overwhelming states of emotion and
could potentially lead the client to feel retrau-
matized or overpowered by negative emotions.
This could result in an early termination of
treatment and tarnish the efficacy of psychother-
apy. Moreover, if a client feels they will not be
able to control their response to an arousing
state of affect, they will avoid the emotion,
often through a variety of defense mechanisms,
further preventing any future opportunity for
affect reconsolidation.
Recently, memory reconsolidation has been
proposed as the mechanism of change in psy-
chotherapy (Ecker, Ticic, & Hulley, 2012; Lane
et al., 2015; Welling, 2012). Memory reconsoli-
dation is referred to in this article as affect
reconsolidation. This distinction is made be-
cause affect reconsolidation is not a change in
the content of the memory but a change in the
emotional valence and intensity associated with
the memory. The same memory exists, but after
affect reconsolidation, the emotion of the mem-
ory changes, a recollection of the trauma event
will have the same content, but the feelings
3AFFECT REGULATION AND RECONSOLIDATION
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
around that content will be very different. This
article is in agreement—with Lane et al.
(2015)—that memory reconsolidation is the
mechanism of change in psychotherapy, yet fur-
ther proposes that the ability to effectively reg-
ulate affect is a necessary condition for the
secondary process of affect reconsolidation to
occur. Some authors even suggest that affect
regulation is the main component of change in
psychotherapy (Moyal, Cohen, Henik, & An-
holt, 2015). An increasingly amount of empiri-
cal support has demonstrated that a moderate
level of emotional arousal is most effective for
therapeutic change (Carryer & Greenberg,
2010; Corrigan, Fisher, & Nutt, 2011). Thus,
both affect regulation and affect reconsolidation
are equally important components of therapeu-
tic change. Affect regulation is necessary to
titrate emotion to an optimal level of moderate
arousal, whereas affect reconsolidation is im-
portant in transforming the baseline emotional
response once the emotion is activated.
The two mechanisms of affect regulation and
reconsolidation show different neural activation
responses in the brain (Frank et al., 2014;
Ochsner & Gross, 2005). Affect regulation in-
volves the use of higher cortical areas such as
the dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex and ventral
lateral prefrontal cortex, and reduction of
amygdala activation is seen after the regulation
of affect (Buhle et al., 2014). Kohn et al. (2014)
proposed that during affect regulation, the ven-
tral lateral prefrontal cortex engages in cogni-
tive appraisal, which then, through the dorsal
anterior cingulate cortex (ACC; also referred to
anterior mid-cingulate cortex), tempers emo-
tion in subcortical areas such as the amygdala
(Frank et al., 2014; Kim & Hamann, 2007).
Affect reconsolidation, operationalized as ex-
tinction or memory reconsolidation, appears to
involve lower cortical areas, primarily the ven-
tral medial prefrontal cortex and amygdala, and
also the dorsal ACC and hippocampus, to a
lesser extent (Milad & Quirk, 2012). Evidence
suggest that following reconsolidation circuitry
may be altered between ventral medial prefron-
tal cortex and amygdala (Schiller, Kanen, Le-
Doux, Monfils, & Phelps, 2013). Comparison
studies between the up- and downregulation of
emotion have found distinctive neural correlates
for each process (Frank et al., 2014; Ochsner et
al., 2004), distinguishing separate systems for
affect regulation and affect reconsolidation. Af-
fect regulation as a construct has often been
operationalized using the Difficulties in Emo-
tion Regulation Scale (Gratz & Roemer, 2004).
Other measures have been used to measure af-
fect regulation, and all show a relationship be-
tween poor emotion regulation and psychopa-
thology (Aldao et al., 2010). The process of
affect reconsolidation has been measured in the
laboratory setting through changing emotional
memories, and this process can be affected by
stress (Schwabe & Wolf, 2010). Affect recon-
solidation has shown promise as a therapeutic
tool in changing emotional memory (Beckers &
Kindt, 2017). Both affect regulation and affect
reconsolidation have been shown to be positive
contributors to therapeutic change (Linehan et
al., 2006; Schiller et al., 2010) using different
clinical interventions as previously outlined.
Recent approaches, such as the unified pro-
tocol (Barlow et al., 2010; Wilamowska et al.,
2010) and affect phobia therapy (McCullough
& Andrews, 2001), have also begun adding
interventions to increase affect in conjunction
with the more traditional affect regulation strat-
egies with interventions in evoking affect, rep-
resenting a positive step toward integrating psy-
chotherapy. CBT can also utilize affect
regulation and affect reconsolidation methods
(Samoilov & Goldfried, 2000); however, this
approach often insufficient when the feared
emotion is an internalized feeling (Westen,
2000), and emphasizes extinction over affect
reconsolidation. Although these therapies repre-
sent an important step, more can be done to in
creating consistent therapeutic principles. Ther-
apeutic approaches that focus solely on one side
of this dichotomy are based on a false supposi-
tion, because one cannot regulate affect without
first having affect. Furthermore, it is unlikely
that one can one reconsolidate affect if the af-
fect cannot be regulated to a tolerable level of
arousal (Alberini & Ledoux, 2013). Therefore.
placing a more equal emphasis on affect regu-
lation and affect reconsolidation can enhance
the process of psychotherapy.
Affect Regulation
Mindfulness has recently become popular as
an affect regulation skill (Shapiro & Carlson,
2017). This involves a deautomatization of pre-
vious actions, in which the individual is able to
separate their emotional experience from their
4 STEVENS
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
actions (Kang, Gruber, & Gray, 2013). The
ability to be mindful has been shown to directly
help individuals in regulating their emotions
(Arch & Craske, 2006; Jimenez, Niles, & Park,
2010). Through mindfulness, the individual
learns to experience their feelings without au-
tomatically reacting. The individual is then able
to consider alternative strategies for managing
the emotion and possibly implement healthier
behavior. Research shows that regulation of af-
fect involves the use of top-down control
through the orbitofrontal, anterior cingulate,
and prefrontal cortexes to mitigate feelings (Et-
kin, Egner, & Kalisch, 2011; Ochsner & Gross,
2005). These neocortical regions may help in
allowing individuals to consider and implement
alternative actions when feeling emotionally
overwhelmed.
Cognitive therapy acts in a similar way, slow-
ing down the brain to reevaluate thoughts,
which helps to realign one's beliefs on a more
realistic level. Cognitive therapy utilizes in-
creased awareness to engage in conscious re-
flection of thought to evaluate a belief before
moving into action. It helps individuals ques-
tion their assumptions about the world before
believing the worst. Cognitive therapy does not
examine the affect from which the thought is
derived. It examines the individual thoughts in
response to their affect, encouraging individ-
uals to rethink things in a more realistic man-
ner (Beck, 2010). This, in turn, helps individ-
uals see the world in a potentially more
realistic light, which will enable them to ef-
fectively change their behavior and further
prevent thoughts that enable negative states of
affect. Cognitive therapy, in the pure sense, is
not an intervention to change basic affect;
rather, the goal is regulating affect through
managing thinking and behavior in response
to emotion (Kazdin, 2007). Research also ex-
ists to suggest that affect regulation can be an
implicit process as well (Braunstein, Gross, &
Ochsner, 2017). For example, a therapist may
simultaneously experience a client's emotion
as the client is feeling the emotion, during
which the therapist can model regulating this
emotion, thus providing an implicit model
through which the client can regulate their
emotion (Gyurak, Gross, & Etkin, 2011). All
of this happens without words and is similar
to the reflective functioning processes of reg-
ulating emotion that occurs between a care-
giver and their child (Fonagy & Target, 1997;
Schore, 2016; Siegel, 2012). However, to
change the basic affect, an intervention to
address the emotion(s), and not just the re-
sulting symptoms of the emotion(s), is need-
ed. Affect reconsolidation is such an interven-
tion.
Affect Reconsolidation: Moving Toward
Primary Affect
Greenberg and Pascual-Leone (2006) theo-
rized a distinction between primary emotions
and secondary emotions. Primary emotions
occur during the initial response to an event,
whereas secondary emotion(s) are pathologi-
cal reactions to the avoidance of primary
emotions (Greenberg & Safran, 1989). The
continual avoidance of a primary emotion re-
sults in a secondary emotion that exists indef-
initely, and this can be considered a state of
psychopathology. Greenberg and Pascual-
Leone suggested that one must first explore
secondary emotions and then progress to ad-
dressing primary emotions. For example, one
might feel continually hopeless after the death
of their spouse. The secondary emotion of
hopelessness exists as a result of the avoid-
ance of the primary emotion of sadness that
resulted from the death. The widower may
avoid their sadness, perhaps because they feel
unable to cope with grief or are afraid to
acknowledge the death and the resulting
changes. This causes a perpetual state of suf-
fering in which the individual will always feel
hopeless if they are unable to access their
sadness and grief. The hopelessness is the
resulting feeling of the ongoing avoidance of
the sadness. Essentially, the widower lives in
denial and in a continuous state of suffering.
Ostensibly, no one would knowingly choose
to suffer, but the suffering results because the
individual avoids the painful primary emo-
tion. Clients may be unaware of these primary
emotions or be actively defending themselves
against experiencing these primary emotions,
afraid they will be unable to cope with the
feelings once activated. The client may fear
that they will not be able to regulate this
primary emotion effectively and will act out
in harmful ways, such as through substance
use or self-harm. In some cases, clients may
fear falling deeper into a depression or psy-
5AFFECT REGULATION AND RECONSOLIDATION
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
chotic state. Lastly, the client may fear shame
or judgment of the self that comes from hav-
ing certain feelings. These fears are not un-
grounded, and the client must learn the affect
regulation skills referred to above prior to
accessing these challenging feelings. Fosha
(2005) recommended starting with the most
apparent feelings and talking about these feel-
ings in order to eventually understand deeper
feelings, moving the client to increasingly
core levels of affect. Slowly accessing feel-
ings and regulating them accordingly will
give the client confidence that they can con-
tinue to experience intense feelings and effec-
tively cope with these feelings as they arise.
As the client gains confidence in their ability
to feel more and trusts the therapist to help
regulate and support them, the client will
move closer to the primary emotions to then
reconsolidate the affect. In achieving this pro-
cess, the client develops a trust in the thera-
pist to help regulate their affect as the emo-
tions arise. In this sense, it can be understood
why outcome studies of psychotherapy show
that a strong therapeutic relationship is highly
correlated with successful treatment (Flück-
iger, Del Re, Wampold, Symonds, & Horvath,
2012; Horvath, Del Re, Flückiger, & Sy-
monds, 2011; Krupnick et al., 1996).
In order for clients to access such primary
emotions for affect reconsolidation, both the
therapist and client must engage in an increas-
ing conscious reflection of emotion to help
move the client closer to the primary affect
(Lane et al., 2015). The process of deepening
emotion is facilitated by the therapist's reflec-
tion of the client's emotion. This reflection
heightens the emotion and may engage mirror
neurons (de Vignemont & Singer, 2006). The
emotion becomes more conscious and intensi-
fies when another individual reflects the emo-
tion back to the person (Hatfield, Cacioppo, &
Rapson, 1994; Lundqvist & Dimberg, 1995).
This is exemplified in the classical Rogerian
paraphrasing intervention, "It seems your say-
ing/feeling." This reflection of the client's feel-
ing back to the client intensifies the feeling
within the client and heightens the emotional
experience. This entire process can also happen
on a nonverbal level in the therapeutic process,
as the client expresses a feeling and the therapist
reflects that feeling back in a facial expression.
The client experiences the feeling and being
felt. The therapist, in developing a relationship
with the client, attunes to the client's nonverbal
expression, which, in turn, deepens the level of
affect in the therapy session.
Affect Reconsolidation: Reactivation
Reactivation of emotion is likely to be incon-
sistent across client type, and research suggests
that one's attachment style (Stevens, 2017) and
personality (Dimaggio & Lysaker, 2018) affects
one's ability to recognize emotions. Therapists'
emotional attunement and nonjudgmental ac-
ceptance of their clients' emotions will help
enable the client to express deeper levels of
emotion. However, this is often a challenging
process, as some clients may struggle to recog-
nize their feelings or engage in psychological
defenses so as not to experience such uncom-
fortable emotions. Here, the aforementioned af-
fect regulation strategies will be necessary to
employ so that the client can gain confidence in
coping with his or her emotions. Together, the
therapist and client will focus attention on rec-
ognizing and reactivating emotional experi-
ences. This will involve an awareness of emo-
tion. Some authors consider emotional
awareness as its own separate mechanism of
change (Greenberg & Pascual-Leone, 2006).
This article includes emotional awareness as
part of the affect regulation and affect recon-
solidation processes and not as a separate mech-
anism of change, because emotional awareness
does not specifically change affect. Consider an
individual with posttraumatic stress disorder
(PTSD) who continually is aware of their emo-
tions through trauma flashbacks or nightmares;
solely increasing emotional awareness here
would not lead to therapeutic change. Yet emo-
tional awareness is still an important first step
for affect regulation and reconsolidation to be-
gin. Multiple studies have implicated the ACC
and the insula that compromise the salient net-
work as important brain areas involved in the
awareness of emotion (Craig, 2009; Gu, Hof,
Friston, & Fan, 2013; Ibañez, Gleichgerrcht, &
Manes, 2010; Medford & Critchley, 2010;
Menon & Uddin, 2010). Research indicates that
feeling states first arise in the insula and are then
increasingly organized in the ACC as the feel-
ing progresses to an increasingly conscious state
(Medford & Critchley, 2010). As awareness of
the feeling increases, the ACC, along with other
6 STEVENS
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
neocortical brain regions, can act together in
regulating the emotion to achieve a moderate
level of arousal (Etkin et al., 2011; Goldin,
McRae, Ramel, & Gross, 2008; Ochsner &
Gross, 2005). The ACC primarily selects where
to direct attention (Compton, 2003; Elliott &
Dolan, 1998; Lane, Fink, Chau, & Dolan, 1997)
and may have the capacity to direct attention in
downregulating emotion or in upregulating
emotion for reconsolidation. Some research
suggests that the ACC acts as a choice point for
the suppression or conscious awareness of emo-
tion (Anderson et al., 2004; Stevens, 2016).
Given that the brain has the capacity to divert
attention when faced with difficult emotion,
highlights the importance of regulating affect
prior to affect reconsolidation, for if the client is
not confident in their ability to cope with the
upsetting emotion(s) that is often activated in
order for affect reconsolidation to occur, they
may redirect their attention elsewhere. As in the
previous example of the death of a spouse, the
widower must first cope with the feelings of
hopelessness to then access the feeling of
deeper sadness. If they feel that they cannot
cope with the emotion, they may suppress or
deny the sadness.
Attention selection and affect regulation may
work together in managing the secondary emo-
tions before accessing and reconsolidating the
primary emotions. Recent research suggests that
selective attention and affect regulation may be
related processes. Malinowski (2013) suggested
that improved Stroop performance among med-
itators is due to their increased ability to regu-
late affect (the Stroop task is a selective atten-
tion task, in which the participant is asked to
read color words that appear in colors incongru-
ent with the name of the color). Further, Mali-
nowski cited evidence that emotional regulation
and response inhibition are simultaneously im-
proved through mindfulness training (Sahdra et
al., 2011). More specifically, Teper and Inzlicht
(2013) found that emotional acceptance medi-
ates the relationship between meditation and
Stroop performance. Shifting attention toward
the emotion that was previously ignored creates
deautomatization (Kang et al., 2013), in which
the direct link between emotion and action is
severed. This involves the ability to mindfully
step back, accept the emotion instead of auto-
matically acting to control it, and then engage in
a different response selection. Deautomatization
is important in both affect regulation and affect
reconsolidation and has been offered as a gen-
eral therapy change principle (Hoffart & Hof-
fart, 2014). One must be able to simultaneously
regulate affect for a different response selection,
while also staying with uncomfortable emotions
in order to reconsolidate the emotion. In the
case of the widower, at this point, the widower
attends to and copes with hopelessness, allow-
ing further access to the feelings of sadness.
This is not easy because, often when experienc-
ing a difficult emotion, our instinct is to avoid
the emotion. Selectively attending to negative
emotions may initially be counterintuitive and
difficult for clients to understand. Why would a
client want to experience such negative affect if
they do not understand the positive benefits of
affect reconsolidation?
Affect Reconsolidation: Transformation
Research demonstrates that one must experi-
ence an emotion in order to reconsolidate the
emotion (Gorman & Roose, 2011; Schiller et
al., 2010; Shallcross, Troy, Boland, & Mauss,
2010); hence, the individual must consciously
experience the negative affect in order to recon-
solidate it. As Pascual-Leone and Greenberg
(2007) demonstrated in their research, if the
affect is truly to be reconsolidated, the "only
way out is through." The negative memory or
experience is retrieved and felt, and then a new
feeling emerges, in which the memory content
does not change but the emotion memory is
rewritten. In the previous example, the widower
experiences the sadness of the spouse's death
and a new feeling of acceptance, perhaps even a
feeling of gratitude for the relationship, replaces
the sadness. The content of memory surround-
ing the actual death does not change, just the
emotional meaning of the event. This may be
easier to conceptualize with positive memories.
Suppose you win an award—in the days after,
you may experience a positive feeling of pride
every time you recall that memory. Then over
time, you notice that the memory no longer
brings the same emotional valence with it, as it
has been reconsolidated. You may still feel
some pride for having won the award, but the
feeling of pride is diminished compared with
the initial reexperiencing of that pride. We still
have the memory, but the emotional valence
that comes with the memory slowly erodes
7AFFECT REGULATION AND RECONSOLIDATION
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
through affect reconsolidation. If the memory
was infrequently or never recollected, than
when it is recollected, it will have a high level
of emotional intensity. Even though the mem-
ory is from an event that took place years ago,
it creates a strong affect response because it was
never reconsolidated. In this scenario, you have
won an award and felt pride, but never recol-
lected the memory, perhaps because you be-
came distracted with other life events. If you
then recollect the memory, you will experience
a surprising level of pride. However, this is less
frequent with positive memories because our
brain naturally wants to recollect positive mem-
ories/feelings, as it makes us feel good. We are
more likely to repress negative memories, and
we find ourselves surprised by our sadness
when recollecting a sad memory. For example,
when driving and hearing a particular song
linked to a distressing event, inevitably we are
stuck feeling sad.
Once previous feelings are reactivated, re-
consolidation can occur; this is a delicate pro-
cess, as the client needs to experience enough of
the emotion for reconsolidation, but too much
can overwhelm or retraumatize the client, which
is why a moderate level of arousal is most
effective for therapeutic change (Carryer &
Greenberg, 2010; Corrigan et al., 2011). Again,
it is important to be titrating affect throughout
the reconsolidation process to prevent the client
from becoming inundated with feelings. At a
moderate level, affect can be reconsolidated
through replacing the previous experience with
another experience. For example, if a client has
carried around the experience of feeling un-
loved, in this moment, they would feel loved.
Schiller et al. (2010) illustrated how a new
experience is important when the emotional
memory is reactivated for affect reconsolidation
to occur. They conditioned subjects to have an
adverse reaction to a neutral stimulus, by pair-
ing an electric shock with yellow or blue
squares. Their research found that in order to
reconsolidate memory, extinction training has
to take place within a window of time wherein
the adverse stimulus is reactivated. If the train-
ing takes place outside this window when the
adverse stimulus is not reactivated, then recon-
solidation does not occur. This supports previ-
ous clinical evidence (Bridges, 2006) as to why
emotion has to be reactivated in order for affect
reconsolidation to occur. It is not enough for the
therapist to provide clients with a new experi-
ence; this new experience must take place when
the old emotional memory is activated in order
for affect reconsolidation to occur.
The nature of the new experience will vary
depending upon the client's needs. In the Schil-
ler et al. (2010) study, in order to extinguish
their fear, the subject needed to experience that
same blue or yellow square that produced the
fear (reactivation) in a context in which no
adverse shock was given (i.e., a new experi-
ence). Providing this new experience is an es-
sential part of the reconsolidation process—
without it, one may just reinforce the previous,
old emotional memory (Sevenster, Beckers, &
Kindt, 2012). A similar paradigm is used in
exposure therapy for phobias, in which a fear
stimulus is experienced in a safe context. The
fear response is reactivated while the individual
is in a safe emotional context, which provides a
new emotional experience, thus reconsolidating
the affect and ameliorating the phobia. The pro-
cess becomes more nebulous when the feared
emotion is not readily activated by an external
stimulus. For example, an individual with
PTSD may not have any clear external cues that
will activate their trauma, yet the affect recon-
solidation process works the same once the in-
ternal emotional memories are accessible.
Transforming fear into safety might be the pro-
totypical example of affect reconsolidation.
Transforming shame into acceptance, or hate
into compassion, could be more challenging,
and little research exists on how the therapist
facilitates this transformational process.
Integrating Psychotherapy
A major difference among the various ap-
proaches to psychotherapy rests in whether
emotion should be activated or reduced. As
evidenced above, combining the principles of
affect regulation and affect reconsolidation of-
fers a more holistic treatment approach that
recognizes clients' varying needs and abilities
for accessing and regulating their emotion. Af-
fect regulation strategies are often first needed
to de-escalate negative emotions and prevent
further negative actions. Second, strategies to
increase emotions should be used that deepen
affect, helping the client to regulate more diffi-
cult levels of emotion and to eventually achieve
a moderate level of emotional arousal. Finally,
8 STEVENS
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
in that moderate state of arousal, a new experi-
ence should be offered through which affect
reconsolidation can occur, transforming the
negative emotion into a positive emotion.
However, the organizing principles of affect
regulation and reconsolidation presented here
do not attempt to identify which emotional reg-
ulation strategies are best to employ across dis-
orders, client type, therapeutic style, and so
forth, nor what therapeutic conditions are best
for the reconsolidation of emotion or what pos-
itive emotion should be offered in transforming
the negative emotion. The field of psychother-
apeutic research will continue to identify the
best-practice standards among the large variety
of conditions that exist. Moreover, these princi-
ples do not offer a set timeline for therapy, the
client's incoming coping skills, or affect toler-
ance, and the rate at which the client can man-
age increasingly difficult levels of emotions will
greatly determine their ability for reconsolida-
tion. What these two organizing principles do
offer is a way for clinicians to think about
emotion in psychotherapy and to help clinicians
decide when it may be best to up- or downregu-
late emotion. Some clients may become easily
overwhelmed by their emotions and need more
focus on affect regulation, whereas other clients
may be more disconnected from their emotional
experience and need help accessing emotion for
affect reconsolidation. Identifying affect regu-
lation and affect reconsolidation as organizing
principles in psychotherapy can help to move
the field closer to a unified approach, integrating
the science and practice of psychotherapy.
References
Alberini, C. M., & Ledoux, J. E. (2013). Memory
reconsolidation. Current Biology, 23, R746–R750.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.06.046
Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S.
(2010). Emotion-regulation strategies across psy-
chopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical
Psychology Review, 30, 217–237. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004
Alexander, F., & French, T. M. (1946). Psychoana-
lytic therapy: Principles and application. New
York, NY: Ronald Press.
Anderson, M. C., Ochsner, K. N., Kuhl, B., Cooper, J.,
Robertson, E., Gabrieli, S. W.,...Gabrieli, J. D.
(2004). Neural systems underlying the suppression of
unwanted memories. Science, 303, 232–235. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1089504
Arch, J. J., & Craske, M. G. (2006). Mechanisms of
mindfulness: Emotion regulation following a fo-
cused breathing induction. Behaviour Research
and Therapy, 44, 1849–1858. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1016/j.brat.2005.12.007
Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-
Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H.
(2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious
and nonanxious individuals: A meta-analytic
study. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 1–24. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.1
Barlow, D. H., Farchione, T. J., Fairholme, C. P.,
Ellard, K. K., Boisseau, C. L., Allen, L. B., &
Ehrenreich May, J. T. (2010). Unified protocol for
transdiagnostic treatment of emotional disorders:
Therapist guide. New York, NY: Oxford Univer-
sity Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/med:psych/
9780199772667.001.0001
Beck, J. S. (2010). Cognitive therapy. In I. B. Weiner
& W. E. Craighead (Eds.), Corsini Encyclopedia
of Psychology (pp. 1–3). Hoboken, NJ: John Wi-
ley. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/9780470479216
.corpsy0198
Beck, J. S., Liese, B. S., & Najavits, L. M. (2005).
Cognitive therapy. In R. J. Frances, S. I. Miller, &
A. H. Mack (Eds.), Clinical textbook of addictive
disorders (3rd ed., pp. 473–501). New York, NY:
Guilford Press.
Beckers, T., & Kindt, M. (2017). Memory reconsoli-
dation interference as an emerging treatment for
emotional disorders: Strengths, limitations, chal-
lenges, and opportunities. Annual Review of Clin-
ical Psychology, 13, 99–121. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032816-045209
Berking, M., & Wupperman, P. (2012). Emotion
regulation and mental health: Recent findings, cur-
rent challenges, and future directions. Current
Opinion in Psychiatry, 25, 128–134. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1097/YCO.0b013e3283503669
Bourke, C., Douglas, K., & Porter, R. (2010). Pro-
cessing of facial emotion expression in major de-
pression: A review. Australian and New Zealand
Journal of Psychiatry, 44, 681– 696. http://dx.doi
.org/10.3109/00048674.2010.496359
Braunstein, L. M., Gross, J. J., & Ochsner, K. N.
(2017). Explicit and implicit emotion regulation: A
multi-level framework. Social Cognitive and Af-
fective Neuroscience, 12, 1545–1557. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1093/scan/nsx096
Bridges, M. R. (2006). Activating the corrective
emotional experience. Journal of Clinical Psychol-
ogy, 62, 551–568. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jclp
.20248
Buhle, J. T., Silvers, J. A., Wager, T. D., Lopez, R.,
Onyemekwu, C., Kober, H.,...Ochsner, K. N.
(2014). Cognitive reappraisal of emotion: A meta-
analysis of human neuroimaging studies. Cerebral
9AFFECT REGULATION AND RECONSOLIDATION
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Cortex, 24, 2981–2990. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/
cercor/bht154
Cardi, V., Di Matteo, R., Corfield, F., & Treasure, J.
(2013). Social reward and rejection sensitivity in
eating disorders: An investigation of attentional
bias and early experiences. The World Journal of
Biological Psychiatry, 14, 622– 633. http://dx.doi
.org/10.3109/15622975.2012.665479
Carryer, J. R., & Greenberg, L. S. (2010). Optimal
levels of emotional arousal in experiential therapy
of depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 78, 190–199. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1037/a0018401
Compton, R. J. (2003). The interface between emo-
tion and attention: A review of evidence from
psychology and neuroscience. Behavioral and
Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 2, 115–129.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1534582303002002003
Cooper, M. L., Frone, M. R., Russell, M., & Mudar,
P. (1995). Drinking to regulate positive and nega-
tive emotions: A motivational model of alcohol
use. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
69, 990–1005. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-
3514.69.5.990
Corrigan, F. M., Fisher, J. J., & Nutt, D. J. (2011).
Autonomic dysregulation and the window of tol-
erance model of the effects of complex emotional
trauma. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 25, 17–
25. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0269881109354930
Craig, A. D. (2009). How do you feel—now? The
anterior insula and human awareness. Nature Re-
views Neuroscience, 10, 59–70. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1038/nrn2555
Daros, A. R., Uliaszek, A. A., & Ruocco, A. C.
(2014). Perceptual biases in facial emotion recog-
nition in borderline personality disorder. Person-
ality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment,
5, 79– 87. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/per0000056
de Vignemont, F., & Singer, T. (2006). The empathic
brain: How, when and why? Trends in Cognitive
Sciences, 10, 435– 441. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j
.tics.2006.08.008
Dimaggio, G., & Lysaker, P. H. (2018). A pragmatic
view of disturbed self-reflection in personality dis-
orders: Implications for psychotherapy. Journal of
Personality Disorders, 32, 311–328. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1521/pedi.2018.32.3.311
Driessen, E., Van, H. L., Peen, J., Don, F. J., Twisk,
J. W. R., Cuijpers, P., & Dekker, J. J. M. (2017).
Cognitive-behavioral versus psychodynamic ther-
apy for major depression: Secondary outcomes of
a randomized clinical trial. Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology, 85, 653– 663. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1037/ccp0000207
Duvarci, S., Mamou, C. B., & Nader, K. (2006).
Extinction is not a sufficient condition to prevent
fear memories from undergoing reconsolidation in
the basolateral amygdala. European Journal of
Neuroscience, 24, 249–260. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04907.x
Ecker, B., Ticic, R., & Hulley, L. (2012). Unlocking
the emotional brain: Eliminating symptoms at
their roots using memory reconsolidation. New
York, NY: Routledge.
Elliott, R., & Dolan, R. J. (1998). Activation of
different anterior cingulate foci in association with
hypothesis testing and response selection. Neuro-
Image, 8, 17–29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/nimg
.1998.0344
Etkin, A., Egner, T., & Kalisch, R. (2011). Emotional
processing in anterior cingulate and medial pre-
frontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15,
85–93. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.11
.004
Flückiger, C., Del Re, A. C., Wampold, B. E., Sy-
monds, D., & Horvath, A. O. (2012). How central
is the alliance in psychotherapy? A multilevel lon-
gitudinal meta-analysis. Journal of Counseling
Psychology, 59, 10–17. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
a0025749
Fonagy, P., & Target, M. (1997). Attachment and
reflective function: Their role in self-organization.
Development and Psychopathology, 9, 679–700.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0954579497001399
Fosha, D. (2002). The activation of affective change
processes in AEDP. In J. J. Magnavita (Ed.), Com-
prehensive handbook of psychotherapy. Vol. 1:
Psychodynamic and object relations psychothera-
pies (pp. 309–344), New York, NY: Wiley. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1037/e318152004-008
Fosha, D. (2005). Emotion, true self, true other, core
state: Toward a clinical theory of affective change
process. Psychoanalytic Review, 92, 513–551.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/prev.2005.92.4.513
Frank, D. W., Dewitt, M., Hudgens-Haney, M.,
Schaeffer, D. J., Ball, B. H., Schwarz, N. F.,...
Sabatinelli, D. (2014). Emotion regulation: Quan-
titative meta-analysis of functional activation and
deactivation. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Re-
views, 45, 202–211. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j
.neubiorev.2014.06.010
Goldin, P. R., McRae, K., Ramel, W., & Gross, J. J.
(2008). The neural bases of emotion regulation:
Reappraisal and suppression of negative emotion.
Biological Psychiatry, 63, 577–586. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.05.031
Gorman, J. M., & Roose, S. P. (2011). The neurobi-
ology of fear memory reconsolidation and psycho-
analytic theory. Journal of the American Psycho-
analytic Association, 59, 1201–1220. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1177/0003065111427724
Gotlib, I. H., Krasnoperova, E., Yue, D. N., & Joor-
mann, J. (2004). Attentional biases for negative
interpersonal stimuli in clinical depression. Jour-
nal of Abnormal Psychology, 113, 127–135. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.113.1.121
10 STEVENS
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Gratz, K. L., & Roemer, L. (2004). Multidimensional
assessment of emotion regulation and dysregula-
tion: Development, factor structure, and initial val-
idation of the difficulties in emotion regulation
scale. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral
Assessment, 26, 41–54. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/
B:JOBA.0000007455.08539.94
Greenberg, L. S. (2004). Emotion–focused therapy.
Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy: An Inter-
national Journal of Theory & Practice, 11, 3–16.
Greenberg, L. S., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2006). Emo-
tion in psychotherapy: A practice-friendly research
review. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62, 611–
630. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jclp.20252
Greenberg, L. S., & Safran, J. D. (1989). Emotion in
psychotherapy. American Psychologist, 44, 19–29.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.44.1.19
Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion
regulation: An integrative review. Review of Gen-
eral Psychology, 2, 271–299. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271
Gross, J. J., & Jazaieri, H. (2014). Emotion, emotion
regulation, and psychopathology: An affective sci-
ence perspective. Clinical Psychological Science, 2,
387– 401. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/216770261
4536164
Gu, X., Hof, P. R., Friston, K. J., & Fan, J. (2013).
Anterior insular cortex and emotional awareness.
The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 521,
3371–3388. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cne.23368
Gyurak, A., Gross, J. J., & Etkin, A. (2011). Explicit
and implicit emotion regulation: A dual-process
framework. Cognition and Emotion, 25, 400– 412.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2010.544160
Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Rapson, R. L. (1994).
Emotional contagion. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
University Press.
Hoffart, A., & Hoffart, A. R. (2014). Psychotherapy
integration through general therapy change princi-
ples: Missing the core of psychotherapy? Journal
of Psychotherapy Integration, 24, 263–274. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0038135
Horvath, A. O., Del Re, A. C., Flückiger, C., &
Symonds, D. (2011). Alliance in individual psy-
chotherapy. Psychotherapy, 48, 9–16. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1037/a0022186
Ibañez, A., Gleichgerrcht, E., & Manes, F. (2010).
Clinical effects of insular damage in humans.
Brain Structure & Function, 214, 397– 410. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00429-010-0256-y
Jimenez, S. S., Niles, B. L., & Park, C. L. (2010). A
mindfulness model of affect regulation and depres-
sive symptoms: Positive emotions, mood regula-
tion expectancies, and self-acceptance as regula-
tory mechanisms. Personality and Individual
Differences, 49, 645– 650. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1016/j.paid.2010.05.041
Kang, Y., Gruber, J., & Gray, J. R. (2013). Mindfulness
and de-automatization. Emotion Review, 5, 192–201.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1754073912451629
Kazdin, A. E. (2007). Mediators and mechanisms of
change in psychotherapy research. Annual Review
of Clinical Psychology, 3, 1–27. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091432
Kim, S. H., & Hamann, S. (2007). Neural correlates
of positive and negative emotion regulation. Jour-
nal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 776–798.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.5.776
Kohn, N., Eickhoff, S. B., Scheller, M., Laird, A. R.,
Fox, P. T., & Habel, U. (2014). Neural network of
cognitive emotion regulation—An ALE meta-
analysis and MACM analysis. NeuroImage, 87,
345–355. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage
.2013.11.001
Krupnick, J. L., Sotsky, S. M., Simmens, S., Moyer,
J., Elkin, I., Watkins, J., & Pilkonis, P. A. (1996).
The role of the therapeutic alliance in psychother-
apy and pharmacotherapy outcome: Findings in
the National Institute of Mental Health Treatment
of Depression Collaborative Research Program.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
64, 532–539. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X
.64.3.532
Lane, R. D., Fink, G. R., Chau, P. M. L., & Dolan,
R. J. (1997). Neural activation during selective
attention to subjective emotional responses. Neu-
roreport: An International Journal for the Rapid
Communication of Research in Neuroscience, 8,
3969–3972. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00001756-
199712220-00024
Lane, R. D., Ryan, L., Nadel, L., & Greenberg, L.
(2015). Memory reconsolidation, emotional
arousal, and the process of change in psychother-
apy: New insights from brain science. Behavioral
and Brain Sciences, 38, e1. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1017/S0140525X14000041
Leichsenring, F., Salzer, S., Jaeger, U., Kächele, H.,
Kreische, R., Leweke, F.,...Leibing, E. (2009).
Short-term psychodynamic psychotherapy and
cognitive-behavioral therapy in generalized anxi-
ety disorder: A randomized, controlled trial. The
American Journal of Psychiatry, 166, 875– 881.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09030441
Linehan, M. M., Comtois, K. A., Murray, A. M.,
Brown, M. Z., Gallop, R. J., Heard, H. L.,...
Lindenboim, N. (2006). Two-year randomized
controlled trial and follow-up of dialectical behav-
ior therapy vs therapy by experts for suicidal be-
haviors and borderline personality disorder. Ar-
chives of General Psychiatry, 63, 757–766. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.63.7.757
Lundqvist, L.-O., & Dimberg, U. (1995). Facial ex-
pressions are contagious. Journal of Psychophysi-
ology, 9, 203–211.
11AFFECT REGULATION AND RECONSOLIDATION
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Malinowski, P. (2013). Neural mechanisms of atten-
tional control in mindfulness meditation. Frontiers
in Neuroscience, 7, 8. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/
fnins.2013.00008
McCullough, L., & Andrews, S. (2001). Assimilative
integration: Short term dynamic psychotherapy for
treating affectphobias. Clinical Psychology: sci-
ence and practice, 8, 82–97.
Medford, N., & Critchley, H. D. (2010). Conjoint
activity of anterior insular and anterior cingulate
cortex: Awareness and response. Brain Structure
& Function, 214, 535–549. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1007/s00429-010-0265-x
Mennin, D., & Farach, F. (2007). Emotion and evolving
treatments for adult psychopathology. Clinical Psy-
chology: Science and Practice, 14, 329–352. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2850.2007.00094.x
Menon, V., & Uddin, L. Q. (2010). Saliency, switch-
ing, attention and control: A network model of
insula function. Brain Structure & Function, 214,
655– 667. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00429-010-
0262-0
Milad, M. R., & Quirk, G. J. (2012). Fear extinction
as a model for translational neuroscience: Ten
years of progress. Annual Review of Psychology,
63, 129–151. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev
.psych.121208.131631
Moyal, N., Cohen, N., Henik, A., & Anholt, G. E.
(2015). Emotion regulation as a main mechanism
of change in psychotherapy. Behavioral and Brain
Sciences, 38, e18. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/
S0140525X14000259
Nader, K., & Hardt, O. (2009). A single standard for
memory: The case for reconsolidation. Nature Re-
views Neuroscience, 10, 224–234. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1038/nrn2590
Ochsner, K. N., & Gross, J. J. (2005). The cognitive
control of emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,
9, 242–249. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005
.03.010
Ochsner, K. N., Ray, R. D., Cooper, J. C., Robertson,
E. R., Chopra, S., Gabrieli, J. D., & Gross, J. J.
(2004). For better or for worse: Neural systems
supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation
of negative emotion. NeuroImage, 23, 483– 499.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.06
.030
Pascual-Leone, A., & Greenberg, L. S. (2007). Emo-
tional processing in experiential therapy: Why "the
only way out is through." Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology, 75, 875– 887. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.75.6.875
Robins, C. J., Ivanoff, A. M., & Linehan, M. M.
(2001). Dialectical behavior therapy. In W. J.
Livesley (Ed.), Handbook of personality disor-
ders: Theory, research, and treatment (pp. 437–
459). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Rottenberg, J. E., & Johnson, S. L. (2007). Emotion
and psychopathology: Bridging affective and clin-
ical science. Washington, DC: American Psycho-
logical Association. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
11562-000
Sahdra, B. K., MacLean, K. A., Ferrer, E., Shaver,
P. R., Rosenberg, E. L., Jacobs, T. L.,...Saron,
C. D. (2011). Enhanced response inhibition during
intensive meditation training predicts improve-
ments in self-reported adaptive socioemotional
functioning. Emotion, 11, 299–312. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1037/a0022764
Samoilov, A., & Goldfried, M. R. (2000). Role of
emotion in cognitive-behavior therapy. Clinical
Psychology: Science and Practice, 7, 373–385.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/clipsy.7.4.373
Schiller, D., Kanen, J. W., LeDoux, J. E., Monfils,
M. H., & Phelps, E. A. (2013). Extinction during
reconsolidation of threat memory diminishes pre-
frontal cortex involvement. Proceedings of the Na-
tional Academy of Sciences of the United States of
America, 110, 20040–20045. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1073/pnas.1320322110
Schiller, D., Monfils, M. H., Raio, C. M., Johnson,
D. C., Ledoux, J. E., & Phelps, E. A. (2010).
Preventing the return of fear in humans using
reconsolidation update mechanisms. Nature, 463,
49–53. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nature08637
Schiller, D., & Phelps, E. A. (2011). Does reconsoli-
dation occur in humans? Frontiers in Behavioral
Neuroscience, 5, 24. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/
fnbeh.2011.00024
Schore, A. N. (2016). Affect regulation and the origin
of the self: The neurobiology of emotional devel-
opment. New York, NY: Routledge.
Schwabe, L., Nader, K., & Pruessner, J. C. (2014).
Reconsolidation of human memory: Brain mecha-
nisms and clinical relevance. Biological Psychia-
try, 76, 274–280. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j
.biopsych.2014.03.008
Schwabe, L., & Wolf, O. T. (2010). Stress impairs
the reconsolidation of autobiographical memories.
Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 94, 153–
157. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2010.05.001
Sevenster, D., Beckers, T., & Kindt, M. (2012). Re-
trieval per se is not sufficient to trigger reconsoli-
dation of human fear memory. Neurobiology of
Learning and Memory, 97, 338–345. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2012.01.009
Shallcross, A. J., Troy, A. S., Boland, M., & Mauss,
I. B. (2010). Let it be: Accepting negative emo-
tional experiences predicts decreased negative af-
fect and depressive symptoms. Behaviour Re-
search and Therapy, 48, 921–929. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1016/j.brat.2010.05.025
Shapiro, S. L., & Carlson, L. E. (2017). The art and
science of mindfulness: Integrating mindfulness
into psychology and the helping professions (2nd
12 STEVENS
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychological As-
sociation. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0000022-000
Shedler, J. (2010). The efficacy of psychodynamic
psychotherapy. American Psychologist, 65, 98–
109. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0018378
Siegel, D. J. (2012). The developing mind: How
relationships and the brain interact to shape who
we are. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Sloan, E., Hall, K., Moulding, R., Bryce, S., Mildred,
H., & Staiger, P. K. (2017). Emotion regulation as
a transdiagnostic treatment construct across anxi-
ety, depression, substance, eating and borderline
personality disorders: A systematic review. Clini-
cal Psychology Review, 57, 141–163. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2017.09.002
Stevens, F. L. (2016). The anterior cingulate cortex in
psychopathology and psychotherapy: Effects on
awareness and repression of affect. Neuropsycho-
analysis, 18, 53– 68. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
15294145.2016.1149777
Stevens, F. L. (2017). Authenticity: A mediator in the
relationship between attachment style and affec-
tive functioning. Counselling Psychology Quar-
terly, 30, 392– 414. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
09515070.2016.1176010
Teper, R., & Inzlicht, M. (2013). Meditation, mind-
fulness and executive control: The importance of
emotional acceptance and brain-based perfor-
mance monitoring. Social Cognitive and Affective
Neuroscience, 8, 85–92. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/
scan/nss045
Tracy, J. L., Klonsky, E. D., & Proudfit, G. H.
(2014). How affective science can inform clinical
science: An introduction to the special series on
emotions and psychopathology. Clinical Psycho-
logical Science, 2, 371–386. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1177/2167702614537627
Verduyn, P., & Lavrijsen, S. (2015). Which emotions
last longest and why: The role of event importance
and rumination. Motivation and Emotion, 39, 119–
127. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11031-014-9445-y
Welling, H. (2012). Transformative emotional se-
quence: Towards a common principle of change.
Journal of Psychotherapy Integration, 22, 109–
136. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0027786
Werner, K., & Gross, J. J. (2010). Emotion regulation
and psychopathology: A conceptual framework. In
A. M. Kring & D. M. Sloan (Eds.), Emotion reg-
ulation and psychopathology: A transdiagnostic
approach to etiology and treatment (pp. 13–37).
New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Westen, D. (2000). Commentary: Implicit and Emo-
tional Processes in Cognitive Behavioral Therapy.
Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 7,
386–390.
Wilamowska, Z. A., Thompson-Hollands, J.,
Fairholme, C. P., Ellard, K. K., Farchione, T. J., &
Barlow, D. H. (2010). Conceptual background,
development, and preliminary data from the uni-
fied protocol for transdiagnostic treatment of emo-
tional disorders. Depression and Anxiety, 27, 882–
890. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/da.20735
Wiser, S., & Goldfried, M. R. (1993). Comparative
study of emotional experiencing in psychodynamic-
interpersonal and cognitive-behavioral therapies.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61,
892– 895. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.61.5
.892
Received February 14, 2018
Revision received June 8, 2018
Accepted June 18, 2018 䡲
13AFFECT REGULATION AND RECONSOLIDATION
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
... Many studies have demonstrated the MR process with target learnings that involve no emotion, such as memory of a spatial arrangement of emotionally neutral objects, procedural memory of finger movements, or declarative/episodic memory of meaningless syllables. These research facts also mean that the MR mechanism is misrepresented by accounts that depict it as an inherently emotional or affective process (e.g., Lane et al. 2015;Stevens 2019;Welling 2012). ...
- Bruce Ecker
- Sara K. Bridges
Memory reconsolidation research by neuroscientists has demonstrated the erasure of emotional learnings. This article reviews these historic findings and how they translate directly into therapeutic application to provide the clinical field with an empirically confirmed process of transformational change. Psychotherapists' early use of this new, transtheoretical knowledge indicates a strong potential for significant advances in both the effectiveness of psychotherapy and the unification of its many diverse systems. The erasure process consists of the creation of certain critical experiences required by the brain, and it neither dictates nor limits the experiential methods that therapists can use to facilitate the needed experiences. This article explains memory reconsolidation, delineates the empirically confirmed process, illustrates it in a case example of long-term depression, indicates the evidence supporting the hypothesis that this process is responsible for transformational change in any therapy sessions, describes the differing mechanisms underlying transformational change versus incremental change, and reports extensive clinical evidence that the basis and cause of most of the problems and symptoms presented by therapy clients are emotional learnings, that is, emotionally laden mental models, or schemas, in semantic memory.
... rekonsolidace paměti doložený neurovědou teprve nedávno (Beckers, Kindt, 2017;Lane et al., 2015). Stevens (2019) ve svém článku ukazuje, jak se vědomá regulace emocí a korektivní zkušenosti mohou v psychoterapii doplňovat, a navrhuje obě tyto možnosti práce s emocemi v psychoterapii integrovat. ...
- Jan Benda
Background. Frequent comorbidity of mental disorders together with the latest findings in neurobiology have been drawing attention of health professionals over the past years to so-called transdiagnostic factors, including, but not limited to difficulties in emotion regulation and self-compassion. It seems that these factors play a significant role in the etiology of many mental disorders as well as in the maintenance of mental health and well-being. Objectives. The aim of this thesis was to theoretically discuss and scientifically verify the expected relationship between parental emotional warmth in childhood, self-compassion and emotion regulation. Sample and procedure. 440 adult respondents (141 men and 299 women) completed the questionnaire "My Memories of Upbringing"- Short Form (s-EMBU), the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale-Short Form (DERS-SF-CZ) and the Self-Compassion Scale (SCS-CZ) together with several demographic questions in an online survey. Statistical analysis. Descriptive statistics and the Pearson's correlation coefficients were calculated using IBM SPSS Statistics-23 software. The effect of gender and education on key variables was tested using two-way multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA). Gender differences in correlations were compared using Fisher's r-to-z transformation. We also performed mediation analysis according to Baron and Kenny's procedure. Results. The results revealed significant correlations between the observed variables and showed that self-compassion was a significant mediator of the relationship between parental emotional warmth in childhood and difficulties in emotion regulation. The correlations of parental emotional warmth in childhood with self-compassion as well as with difficulties in emotion regulation were significantly higher in men than in women. Study limitations. This study relied exclusively on self-report measures. Respondents evaluated their parents' warmth only retrospectively.
- Paola Miano
- Gaia Di Salvo
- Massimo Lavaggi
Seeking help and assistance can prove to be a frustrating experience for intersex individuals: physicians and psychologists can be misinformed about variations of sex characteristics (VSC) and their practice may have a detrimental effect on the physical and psychological wellbeing of individuals with VSC. This paper focuses on how previous negative experiences, such as early genital cosmetic surgery and unaware clinical management, could negatively affect psychotherapeutic relationships. Its principal aims are to: 1) identify which elements in clinical management of VSC may cause difficulties in building a therapeutic alliance and 2) identify which elements may promote the construction and reinforcement of the therapeutic alliance, even when a patient's trust has been damaged by previous negative experiences. In order to explore the psychotherapeutic process with specific regard to the building of the therapeutic alliance, we report a case study whose subject is a 32-year-old man who sought legal advice for his sex reassignment process and began a brief supportive psychotherapeutic treatment. Roberto, the subject of this study, was born with VSC, was assigned the female gender and underwent genital surgery at birth. In his early childhood, he developed a male gender identity. Whenever Roberto reflected on the genital surgery that he had undergone at birth, anger and frustration pervaded his psychotherapy sessions and made it difficult to build a therapeutic alliance. However, it was possible to support Roberto in redirecting these negative affects, helping him to process them, build a therapeutic alliance, and benefit from a positive and trusting therapeutic relationship.
- Ruth Metten
Dieses Kapitel klärt die Frage, ob bei Gedächtnis-Updates, die Erinnerungen an Erfahrungen überschreiben, in der Tat das Wissen darum verloren geht, was ursprünglich geschah.
- Laura Martin Braunstein
- James J Gross
- Kevin N Ochsner
The ability to adaptively regulate emotion is essential for mental and physical well-being. How should we organize the myriad ways people attempt to regulate their emotions? We explore the utility of a framework that distinguishes among four fundamental classes of emotion regulation strategies. The framework describes each strategy class in terms their behavioral characteristics, underlying psychological processes, and supporting neural systems. A key feature of this multi-level framework is its conceptualization of the psychological processes in terms of two orthogonal dimensions that describe (1) the nature of the emotion regulation goal (ranging from to implicit to explicit) and (2) the nature of the emotion change process (ranging from more automatic to more controlled). After describing the core elements of the framework, we use it to review human and animal research on the neural bases of emotion regulation and to suggest key directions for future research on emotion regulation.
Experimental research on emotional memory reconsolidation interference, or the induction of amnesia for previously established emotional memory, has a long tradition, but the potential of that research for the development of novel interventions to treat psychological disorders has been recognized only recently. Here we provide an overview of basic research and clinical studies on emotional memory reconsolidation interference. We point out specific advantages of interventions based on memory reconsolidation interference over traditional treatment for emotional disorders. We also explain how findings from basic research suggest limitations and challenges to clinical translation that may help to understand why clinical trials have met with mixed success so far and how their success can be increased. In closing, we preview new intervention approaches beyond the induction of amnesia that the phenomenon of memory reconsolidation may afford for alleviating the burden imposed by emotional memories, and we comment on theoretical controversies regarding the nature of memory reconsolidation.
- Francis Leroy Stevens
The awareness of emotion and its subsequent regulation is an essential part of psychopathology and psychotherapy. A body of evidence indicates that the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) plays a key role in accessing and regulating emotion and thus participates both in the conscious awareness and the repression of emotion. Research is examined drawing a distinction between psychopathologies with excessive affect, which exhibit hyperactivity in ACC, and psychopathologies with flat affect, which exhibit a hypoactivity in the rostral ACC. This distinction may support therapeutic interventions that target the up- or down-regulation of emotion dependent on clinical disorder. Specific focus is made to psychoanalytic therapy, being built upon the tenet of accessing repressed emotions, for which the ACC appears functionally significant. The research reviewed provides insight into how the ACC may be a primary region in the suppression and repression of emotion. Suggestions for how the ACC may be activated in different types of psychotherapy are also made.
This online therapist guide is a radical departure from disorder-specific treatments of various emotional disorders, and is designed to be applicable to all anxiety and unipolar mood disorders, as well as other disorders with strong emotional components, such as many somatoform and dissociative disorders. It covers the Unified Protocol (UP), which capitalizes on the contributions made by cognitive-behavioral theorists by distilling and incorporating the common principles of CBT present in all evidenced based protocols for specific emotional disorders, as well as drawing on the field of emotion science for insights into deficits in emotion regulation. It covers the seven modules of UP, and focuses on four core strategies to help patients: becoming mindfully aware of emotional experience; reappraising rigid emotion laden attributions; identifying and preventing behavioral and emotional avoidance; and facilitating exposure to both interoceptive and situational cues associated with emotional experiences.
Given recent attention to emotion regulation as a potentially unifying function of diverse symptom presentations, there is a need for comprehensive measures that adequately assess difficulties in emotion regulation among adults. This paper (a) proposes an integrative conceptualization of emotion regulation as involving not just the modulation of emotional arousal, but also the awareness, understanding, and acceptance of emotions, and the ability to act in desired ways regardless of emotional state; and (b) begins to explore the factor structure and psychometric properties of a new measure, the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS). Two samples of undergraduate students completed questionnaire packets. Preliminary findings suggest that the DERS has high internal consistency, good test–retest reliability, and adequate construct and predictive validity.
A large body of research has implicated difficulties in emotion regulation as central to the development and maintenance of psychopathology. Emotion regulation has therefore been proposed as a transdiagnostic construct or an underlying mechanism in psychopathology. The transdiagnostic role of emotion regulation has yet to be systematically examined within the psychological treatment outcome literature. It can be proposed that if emotion regulation is indeed a transdiagnostic construct central to the maintenance of psychopathology, then changes in emotion regulation difficulties will occur after effective treatment and this will occur for different disorders. We conducted a systematic review, identifying 67 studies that measured changes in both emotion regulation and symptoms of psychopathology following a psychological intervention for anxiety, depression, substance use, eating pathology or borderline personality disorder. Results demonstrated that regardless of the intervention or disorder, both maladaptive emotion regulation strategy use and overall emotion dysregulation were found to significantly decrease following treatment in all but two studies. Parallel decreases were also found in symptoms of anxiety, depression, substance use, eating pathology and borderline personality disorder. These results contribute to the growing body of evidence supporting the conceptualization of emotion regulation as a transdiagnostic construct. The present study discusses the important implications of these findings for the development of unified treatments that target emotion regulation for individuals who present with multiple disorders.
- Francis Leroy Stevens
Attachment has been show to exhibit a strong effect on emotional well-being throughout an individual's lifetime. This study examined individuals' authenticity as a potential mediating variable in the relationship between insecure attachment and affective functioning. Authenticity was examined from multiple perspectives to better define its role as a mediating variable. Results showed that avoidant attachment is a predictor of affective functioning, and that authenticity acts as a mediating variable in this relationship. Anxious attachment showed no direct relationship with affective functioning, yet evidence was found for an indirect pathway from anxious attachment through authenticity on affective functioning. The differing results by attachment style support Mikulincer, Shaver, and Pereg's theory suggesting a two-part model of emotional response style, wherein individuals with avoidant attachment deactivate emotion and individuals with anxious attachment show hyper-activation when coping with emotion. The study examines the construct of authenticity and its importance in affective functioning. Implications for therapists working with clients are made that highlight the importance of authentic relationships in therapy and life.
Source: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/328087359_Affect_Regulation_and_Affect_Reconsolidation_as_Organizing_Principles_in_Psychotherapy
Posted by: stephanstephanfindlaye0270889.blogspot.com
Post a Comment for "Affect Regulation Schore Free Pdf Download"